//
you're reading...

Conservation

Are Fisheries Scientists Ringing the Dinner Bell for Marine Mammals?

Article:

Stansbury, A.L., T. Gotz, V.B. Deecke, and V.M. Janik. 2014. Grey seals use anthropogenic signals from acoustic tags to locate fish: Evidence from a simulated foraging task. Proceedings from the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282: 20141595. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1595

Background:

Grey Seals

Figure 1 - Up close and personal with grey seal teeth!

Figure 1 – Up close and personal with grey seal teeth!

Grey seals are a species of seal that are found only in the North Atlantic. Because of their cute and playful nature, these marine mammals don’t always get the credit they deserve for being ferocious predators.  A grey seal’s diet consists entirely of fish and other oceanic critters, with a single seal consuming as much as 10 pounds of fish per day! Unlike fish which are rarely able to detect sounds greater than 3,000 Hertz (Hz) in the water, many marine mammals including seals, dolphins, and whales are able to detect sounds greater than 160,000 Hz. These higher frequency sounds are often used by marine mammals for communication and location of prey items.

Acoustic Telemetry

Acoustic telemetry is a method of tracking animals that has recently been heavily utilized by fisheries scientists to track the movement of many species of fish. Acoustic telemetry works by capturing a fish, then implanting them with a small tag. This tag is programmed to emit a sound (usually undetectable by the tagged animal) over a regular a time period, which can be detected by acoustic receivers deployed at the study locations. These acoustic receivers are essentially microphones placed in the water which continuously listen for “pings” emitted by the tags. The receivers are able to differentiate between tags allowing scientists to record the presence of individual fish every time they come within range of a receiver. Acoustic telemetry has become such a powerful tool that fisheries scientists are now able to track fish movements in 3D by triangulating the position of a fish relative to multiple acoustic receivers, just like your GPS device triangulates your position relative to multiple satellites! Although acoustic telemetry has proven a very powerful tool in studying the movements of fish, the frequency of the sound emitted by acoustic tags (~69,000 Hz), though usually undetectable by the fish itself, is within the hearing range of many marine mammals, including seals. The researchers in this study attempted to determine whether or not grey seals would learn to recognize the sound emitted by acoustic tags as a source of food and use it as a homing beacon to locate prey in the future.

The Study:

To test whether or not grey seals would learn to home in on acoustic tag signals for locating prey, the researchers used 10 juvenile grey seals.  The seals were brought into captivity before ever entering the sea which guaranteed that the seals had never learned to associate sound with food. The researchers designed a pool that contained 20 boxes along the bottom which seals could poke their heads into and potentially find a fish inside (Figure 2). In each trial, a single seal was released into the testing pool and allowed to freely forage. In the original experiment, one box contained an untagged fish, and one box containing a fish tagged with an acoustic tag. For each new repeated trial, the box containing the tagged and untagged fish would change, so that seals wouldn’t learn to associate geographic location with the presence of the fish. Two control experiments were also conducted to control for the seals potentially using other cues, like scent, to locate the fish. During the first control experiment, no fish were place in any of the 20 boxes. However, acoustic tags (without a fish) were placed in two of the boxes to see if seals would visit the boxes with acoustic tags more quickly and more frequently than untagged boxes. In the second control experiment, every box contained a fish.  However, in eighteen of the twenty boxes, the fish was blocked by a plate, preventing the seals from grabbing it. This second control experiment prevented the seals from being able to locate which box contained an accessible fish using scent. Of the two fish that were accessible to the seals, one of them had an acoustic tag while the other did not.

Figure 2 - Testing pool, drained of water, with foraging boxes along the bottom.

Figure 2 – Testing pool, drained of water, with foraging boxes along the bottom.

Results:

Figure 3 - The amount of time it takes a seal to look in any empty box vs. an acoustic tag box when no fish are present in any of the boxes.

Figure 3 – The amount of time it takes a seal to look in any empty box vs. an acoustic tag box when no fish are present in any of the boxes.

All seals in the study became better at locating fish in the boxes as they were given more trials. The time required to find both tagged and untagged fish in the original experiment decreased as the seals gained more experience in the test pool. This suggests that the seals were able learn how to utilize a number of environmental cues to locate their food more quickly. However, the interesting findings of this study came from the two control experiments. The first control experiment demonstrated that in the absence of scent, seals would move more quickly to acoustic tag boxes in an attempt to find food (Figure 3).

The second control experiment demonstrated that when the scent of fish could not lead the seals to an accessible fish, the seals would utilize the acoustic sound to find accessible prey (Figure 4). Note that in the second trial, even the untagged fish was discovered very quickly in this experiment. The researchers attributed this to the fact that inaccessible fish were reused for each trial, while accessible fish were eaten by the seals and had to be replaced with fresh fish for a subsequent trial. Therefore, seals were likely able to differentiate between the smell of fresh fish vs. reused fish.

Figure 4 - The amount of time it takes a seal to successfully find and obtain an accessible fish when 18/20 boxes have inaccessible fish, 1/20 has an untagged fish, and 1/20 has an acoustic tagged fish.

Figure 4 – The amount of time it takes a seal to successfully find and obtain an accessible fish when 18/20 boxes have inaccessible fish, 1/20 has an untagged fish, and 1/20 has an acoustic tagged fish.

Conclusion:

This study demonstrated that young grey seals have the capacity to associate audible pings emitted by acoustic tags frequently used by fisheries scientists with potential prey. This discovery means that predators in the wild may learn to disproportionately search for and prey on tagged animals. However, it is important to recognize that this study was conducted under controlled lab settings when seals were exposed to a static environment and the presence of an acoustic signal in every trial. It is unclear whether or not marine mammals in the wild would be able to make the connection between acoustic signals and prey item as quickly and efficiently as the test subjects in this study, or if wild animals would give any preference to tagged fish.

Whether or not marine mammals in the wild are able to locate prey that have been tagged by fisheries scientists may be unclear, but the possibility is undeniable. With a steep price tag of usually over $200 per tag, some fisheries scientists have reason to be concerned about marine mammals wreaking havoc on the sample sizes of their acoustic tracking studies; not to mention unforeseen population and behavioral consequences.

Figure 5 - Grey Seal

Figure 5 – Grey Seal

Discussion

2 Responses to “Are Fisheries Scientists Ringing the Dinner Bell for Marine Mammals?”

  1. Interesting study but the proportion of fish with acoustic tags to fish without is nowhere near 50% in the wild. Here, the acoustic stimuli is exaggerated and likely accelerated learning to hunt down acoustically tagged fish. Nonetheless, good to know acoustic pings can be heard by grey seals. Would be interesting to know if they can detect it over natural background noise.

    Posted by Zach Siders | January 25, 2015, 12:21 pm

Trackbacks/Pingbacks

  1. […] time readers may have noticed how often acoustics comes up in the study of the ocean. This is no coincidence and there are myriad reasons why. The most basic, […]

Post a Comment

Instagram

  • by oceanbites 2 months ago
    Happy Earth Day! Take some time today to do something for the planet and appreciate the ocean, which covers 71% of the Earth’s surface.  #EarthDay   #OceanAppreciation   #Oceanbites   #CoastalVibes   #CoastalRI 
  • by oceanbites 3 months ago
    Not all outdoor science is fieldwork. Some of the best days in the lab can be setting up experiments, especially when you get to do it outdoors. It’s an exciting mix of problem solving, precision, preparation, and teamwork. Here is
  • by oceanbites 4 months ago
    Being on a research cruise is a unique experience with the open water, 12-hour working shifts, and close quarters, but there are some familiar practices too. Here Diana is filtering seawater to gather chlorophyll for analysis, the same process on
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on  #oceanbites  we are featuring Hannah Collins  @hannahh_irene  Hannah works with marine suspension feeding bivalves and microplastics, investigating whether ingesting microplastics causes changes to the gut microbial community or gut tissues. She hopes to keep working
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    Leveling up - did you know that crabs have a larval phase? These are both porcelain crabs, but the one on the right is the earlier stage. It’s massive spine makes it both difficult to eat and quite conspicuous in
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Cierra Braga. Cierra works ultraviolet c (UVC) to discover how this light can be used to combat biofouling, or the growth of living things, on the hulls of ships. Here, you
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Elena Gadoutsis  @haysailor  These photos feature her “favorite marine research so far: From surveying tropical coral reefs, photographing dolphins and whales, and growing my own algae to expose it to different
  • by oceanbites 6 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on Oceanbites we are featuring Eliza Oldach. According to Ellie, “I study coastal communities, and try to understand the policies and decisions and interactions and adaptations that communities use to navigate an ever-changing world. Most of
  • by oceanbites 6 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Jiwoon Park with a little photographic help from Ryan Tabata at the University of Hawaii. When asked about her research, Jiwoon wrote “Just like we need vitamins and minerals to stay
  • by oceanbites 7 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on  #Oceanbites  we are featuring  @riley_henning  According to Riley, ”I am interested in studying small things that make a big impact in the ocean. Right now for my master's research at the University of San Diego,
  • by oceanbites 7 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Gabby Stedman. Gabby is interested in interested in understanding how many species of small-bodied animals there are in the deep-sea and where they live so we can better protect them from
  • by oceanbites 7 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Shawn Wang! Shawn is “an oceanographer that studies ocean conditions of the past. I use everything from microfossils to complex computer models to understand how climate has changed in the past
  • by oceanbites 7 months ago
    Today we are highlighting some of our awesome new authors for  #WriterWednesday  Today we have Daniel Speer! He says, “I am driven to investigate the interface of biology, chemistry, and physics, asking questions about how organisms or biological systems respond
  • by oceanbites 8 months ago
    Here at Oceanbites we love long-term datasets. So much happens in the ocean that sometimes it can be hard to tell if a trend is a part of a natural cycle or actually an anomaly, but as we gather more
  • by oceanbites 8 months ago
    Have you ever seen a lobster molt? Because lobsters have exoskeletons, every time they grow they have to climb out of their old shell, leaving them soft and vulnerable for a few days until their new shell hardens. Young, small
  • by oceanbites 9 months ago
    A lot of zooplankton are translucent, making it much easier to hide from predators. This juvenile mantis shrimp was almost impossible to spot floating in the water, but under a dissecting scope it’s features really come into view. See the
  • by oceanbites 9 months ago
    This is a clump of Dead Man’s Fingers, scientific name Codium fragile. It’s native to the Pacific Ocean and is invasive where I found it on the east coast of the US. It’s a bit velvety, and the coolest thing
  • by oceanbites 10 months ago
    You’ve probably heard of jellyfish, but have you heard of salps? These gelatinous sea creatures band together to form long chains, but they can also fall apart and will wash up onshore like tiny gemstones that squish. Have you seen
  • by oceanbites 11 months ago
    Check out what’s happening on a cool summer research cruise! On the  #neslter  summer transect cruise, we deployed a tow sled called the In Situ Icthyoplankton Imaging System. This can take pictures of gelatinous zooplankton (like jellyfish) that would be
  • by oceanbites 11 months ago
    Did you know horseshoe crabs have more than just two eyes? In these juveniles you can see another set in the middle of the shell. Check out our website to learn about some awesome horseshoe crab research.  #oceanbites   #plankton   #horseshoecrabs 
WP2Social Auto Publish Powered By : XYZScripts.com