//
you're reading...

Behavior

Shark Smarts: Can associative learning change shark behavior?

Reference: Mitchell, J. D., et al. “A novel experimental approach to investigate the potential for behavioural change in sharks in the context of depredation.” Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 530 (2020): 151440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2020.151440

Can sharks learn?

North West Cape, Australia. This cape is a peninsula, so is surrounded on three sides by water, and contains a coral reef in addition to a no-take marine reserve. Image by Murray Foubister via Wikimedia Commons.

Most of us are familiar with nature documentaries involving sharks. They often include researchers on a boat throwing chum or bait into the water in an attempt to lure sharks for tagging or observational purposes. This is clearly an effective way to attract sharks and study them in their natural habitat, and it is a far easier method than researchers scouring areas where sharks dwell in the hopes of spotting one. However, through all of these bait-deployment studies, researchers have never really examined if this act of luring sharks repeatedly alters their behavior. Are sharks more likely to swim up to research vessels in areas where bait-deployment studies are repeatedly used? Does the presence of boats make sharks more likely to feed from these bait-deployments? Can sharks learn, over time, that a boat putting out bait means food for them, and act accordingly? These are questions a research team located in western Australia tried to answer.

Developing a bait-deployment system

Researchers chose four study sites in North West Cape, Australia. Two of the sites were in a no-take marine reserve, which are areas where no fishing is allowed to occur. These areas exist across the globe, and are meant to aid in fish conservation. The other two study areas were outside of the no-take marine reserve, where many fishing boats were present.

In order to entice sharks to approach the research vessel, a weighted metal rod that had a bait bag full of crushed fish, as well as two salmon heads attached to it, was lowered into the water at the study sites. A float was attached to the top of the rod to prevent it from sinking to the bottom, and two downward facing cameras were affixed to the rod to ensure sharks were documented as they approached the bait bag and salmon. Researchers released the bait rod three times a day over six days, and quantified the time of arrival for a shark, the time of first feeding, as well as the species of shark present.

An illustration of the bait-deployment system used by researchers in the study. In addition to what is shown, the boat was anchored to the seafloor. Recreated from Mitchell et al. 2019 by Francesca Giammona. Image created in BioRender.com.

 

How did the sharks behave?

A sickefin lemon shark, the most common shark seen during the study. These sharks were so common that 33 were seen during the six day study period out of 63 sharks total. Image by Patrick Quinn-Graham via Wikimedia Commons.

After the six days, there were very clear trends that the time to arrival and time to first feeding for sharks had decreased rapidly, occurring about three times faster at Day 6 compared to Day 1 for both measurements. Four different shark species were observed feeding at the study sites in the fishing areas; the most common species seen feeding was the sicklefin lemon shark. In contrast, only two species approached the bait rod in the marine reserve areas, and these species did not feed. Tiger sharks were the most common in these no-take marine reserve areas. There was a lot of variation between species in regards to the amount of time each took to approach the bait rod and then feed, which indicates that all shark species do not learn or interact with bait in the same way. Overall, blacktip sharks were the fastest to approach the research vessel and feed, while tiger sharks were the slowest to approach.

It was clear that repeating the bait rod deployment over consecutive days had an effect on the sharks. It was even observed that in the later days of the experiment, sharks took less than one minute from the time the bait rod entered the water to feed. The researchers believe this is an example of associative learning, where the sharks associate the research vessel with food, and swim near it until that food becomes available as the bait rod is deployed. This learning likely relies heavily on smell, as the smell of the bait initially drew sharks to the site, but this learning likely also relies on sound. As the research vessel moved through the water, it made a clear noise via the propeller that the sharks could follow, once they learned that the vessel indicated food. This would allow them to feed immediately once the bait was available, as they were already near the research vessel. It is possible that the sharks in the area had already associated the sounds of typical fishing vessels with food, as these boats also throw out bait often. This could explain why so many more feedings occurred outside the marine reserve, where fishing vessels are free to roam, rather than inside the no-take marine reserve, where no fishing usually occurs and the sound of a boat would be unfamiliar to the sharks that usually swim there.

Why does this matter?

This study shows a clear instance of associative learning in sharks, and can help to inform experimental set up for shark researchers who want to study these creatures using a bait-deployment system. It brings to light that sharks can not only learn to associate sensory cues (i.e. sound) outside of what they may normally experience with food, but that each shark species may learn differently in different environments (i.e. in an area with fishing versus an area with no fishing). This work can also educate fishermen who work in these waters, where sharks are common, to the fact that the act of fishing may draw more attention from these predators over time. For the rest of us, this new knowledge about sharks can hopefully shape our mental image of them from one of a predator ruled by only instinct to one that is capable of behavioral change, even after being on the planet for 450 million years.

Discussion

No comments yet.

Post a Comment

Instagram

  • by oceanbites 2 weeks ago
    Not all outdoor science is fieldwork. Some of the best days in the lab can be setting up experiments, especially when you get to do it outdoors. It’s an exciting mix of problem solving, precision, preparation, and teamwork. Here is
  • by oceanbites 1 month ago
    Being on a research cruise is a unique experience with the open water, 12-hour working shifts, and close quarters, but there are some familiar practices too. Here Diana is filtering seawater to gather chlorophyll for analysis, the same process on
  • by oceanbites 2 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on  #oceanbites  we are featuring Hannah Collins  @hannahh_irene  Hannah works with marine suspension feeding bivalves and microplastics, investigating whether ingesting microplastics causes changes to the gut microbial community or gut tissues. She hopes to keep working
  • by oceanbites 3 months ago
    Leveling up - did you know that crabs have a larval phase? These are both porcelain crabs, but the one on the right is the earlier stage. It’s massive spine makes it both difficult to eat and quite conspicuous in
  • by oceanbites 3 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Cierra Braga. Cierra works ultraviolet c (UVC) to discover how this light can be used to combat biofouling, or the growth of living things, on the hulls of ships. Here, you
  • by oceanbites 3 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Elena Gadoutsis  @haysailor  These photos feature her “favorite marine research so far: From surveying tropical coral reefs, photographing dolphins and whales, and growing my own algae to expose it to different
  • by oceanbites 4 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on Oceanbites we are featuring Eliza Oldach. According to Ellie, “I study coastal communities, and try to understand the policies and decisions and interactions and adaptations that communities use to navigate an ever-changing world. Most of
  • by oceanbites 4 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Jiwoon Park with a little photographic help from Ryan Tabata at the University of Hawaii. When asked about her research, Jiwoon wrote “Just like we need vitamins and minerals to stay
  • by oceanbites 4 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  on  #Oceanbites  we are featuring  @riley_henning  According to Riley, ”I am interested in studying small things that make a big impact in the ocean. Right now for my master's research at the University of San Diego,
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Gabby Stedman. Gabby is interested in interested in understanding how many species of small-bodied animals there are in the deep-sea and where they live so we can better protect them from
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    This week for  #WriterWednesday  at  #Oceanbites  we are featuring Shawn Wang! Shawn is “an oceanographer that studies ocean conditions of the past. I use everything from microfossils to complex computer models to understand how climate has changed in the past
  • by oceanbites 5 months ago
    Today we are highlighting some of our awesome new authors for  #WriterWednesday  Today we have Daniel Speer! He says, “I am driven to investigate the interface of biology, chemistry, and physics, asking questions about how organisms or biological systems respond
  • by oceanbites 6 months ago
    Here at Oceanbites we love long-term datasets. So much happens in the ocean that sometimes it can be hard to tell if a trend is a part of a natural cycle or actually an anomaly, but as we gather more
  • by oceanbites 6 months ago
    Have you ever seen a lobster molt? Because lobsters have exoskeletons, every time they grow they have to climb out of their old shell, leaving them soft and vulnerable for a few days until their new shell hardens. Young, small
  • by oceanbites 7 months ago
    A lot of zooplankton are translucent, making it much easier to hide from predators. This juvenile mantis shrimp was almost impossible to spot floating in the water, but under a dissecting scope it’s features really come into view. See the
  • by oceanbites 7 months ago
    This is a clump of Dead Man’s Fingers, scientific name Codium fragile. It’s native to the Pacific Ocean and is invasive where I found it on the east coast of the US. It’s a bit velvety, and the coolest thing
  • by oceanbites 8 months ago
    You’ve probably heard of jellyfish, but have you heard of salps? These gelatinous sea creatures band together to form long chains, but they can also fall apart and will wash up onshore like tiny gemstones that squish. Have you seen
  • by oceanbites 8 months ago
    Check out what’s happening on a cool summer research cruise! On the  #neslter  summer transect cruise, we deployed a tow sled called the In Situ Icthyoplankton Imaging System. This can take pictures of gelatinous zooplankton (like jellyfish) that would be
  • by oceanbites 9 months ago
    Did you know horseshoe crabs have more than just two eyes? In these juveniles you can see another set in the middle of the shell. Check out our website to learn about some awesome horseshoe crab research.  #oceanbites   #plankton   #horseshoecrabs 
  • by oceanbites 9 months ago
    Feeling a bit flattened by the week? So are these summer flounder larvae. Fun fact: flounder larvae start out with their eyes set like normal fish, but as they grow one of their eyes migrates to meet the other and
WP2Social Auto Publish Powered By : XYZScripts.com